Skip to content
Misleading graphs are like comparing apples to oranges. The BFD.

Data visualisations – “graphs”, to the unitiated – have been an incredibly useful tool for cutting through the fog of bias and misunderstanding to get at the facts. One of the most famous early data visualisations was John Snow’s graph plotting every case in the infamous Broad Street cholera outbreak of 1854. Snow’s dot map showed that the outbreak was centred around a single water pump.

A nurse who treated many of the Broad Street patients was Florence Nightingale. Just two years later, her name would be made in the Crimea, where she too, used data visualisation to great effect.

But, in keeping with the adage about “lies, damned lies and statistics”, graphs can be as easily used to mislead and deceive as to inform.

Here are just a few ways graphs can be made to deceive. Forewarned is forearmed. If you learn these tricks and keep an eye out for them, you’re much less likely to be suckered.

Misleading axes.

High-school maths taught us to assume that the X and Y axes of a graph start at zero and count up. But that’s not always the case and when that rule is broken we can easily be fooled.

When KFC used a graph to illustrate the amount of calories in a Chicken Twister compared to, say, a Wendy’s burger, it looks as if the one has half the calories of the other. But note where the X axis starts.

Same data, different origin points for the X-axis. The BFD.

Another infamous example was Michael Mann’s Tiljander Sands graph. The Tiljander et al 2003 core samples of organic material in lake sediments were used as a proxy for global warming. The more organic material, the warmer the climate – so the reasoning went.

Our high school maths also leads us to expect that the Y-axis starts at zero and counts up. Mann published the graphed data, he turned the Y-axis upside-down. The data was still the same but the story it appeared to tell was precisely opposite to the truth.

See what he did, there? The BFD.

Another variation on this theme is, for time-related data, cherry-picking the time period. For example, looking at a company’s share price during only a bad (or good) quarter may give a totally different impression to a five year trend.

Yay! Twitter’s share price is booming! Oh, wait… The BFD.

Similarly, looking at global climate reconstructions over a period of 100 years, one million or 65 million years also gives a very different picture of what may or may not be “normal” or “unprecedented”.

Global temperature anomalies for the past century, one million years and 65 million years. Which do you think the mainstream media would prefer to use?

Misleading representation.

Graphs can look cool, but the way the same data is presented in different graphs can mislead. A bar chart might be better done as a scatter-plot. A 2D pie-chart shown in 3D makes it hard to genuinely compare the data slices.

The 3d chart makes the relative sizes of Items A and C especially hard to judge. The BFD.

A graph with cool-looking judges gavels to show how many appellate judges U.S. presidents confirmed in their first terms is difficult to interpret. Especially when, like the KFC ad, it doesn’t use a baseline of zero. A plain bar graph is much clearer – but probably doesn’t give the impression its publishers wanted.

But… Orange Man Bad! The BFD.

Selection bias.

How you select which data to present can change the apparent picture enormously. For instance, the endlessly-cited claim of “97 percent of scientists…” is based on several studies, each of which is little more than a textbook case of selection bias. For instance, the Doran study only surveyed self-selected respondents (i.e. people who were motivated enough to respond to a mail-out). Of the 10 thousand-plus scientists approached, only 3,146 responded. From those, the study selected a mere 79 or 77 (depending on which of the survey’s two questions they were studying), supposedly “the most specialized and knowledgeable”.

Even with much less subjective data, such as COVID-19 infections, data selection can tell two completely different stories. If you select merely the counties with high infection rates and then break them down by Trump or Clinton votes – oh, those idiot Trumpsters. But, if you compare different-voting counties to the total cumulative infections in the U.S. – well, now it’s the Clintonistas’ turn to look pretty dumb.

Want to make Republicans or Democrats look dumb? Select your data accordingly! The BFD.

Watch out for logarithmic scales.

“Logarithmic” means that each increment on the axis is multiplied from the last by a factor of 10 (e.g. 100, 1,000, 10,000, 100,000 etc.). This can be very useful for presenting big numbers, but it can also be very misleading. Compare these two graphs. Same data – one on a linear scale, the other logarithmic.

A logarithmic scale (left) can make big data easier to show, but can mislead by showing a very different picture to a linear scale (right). The BFD.

These are just a few of the ways genuine data can be used to mislead the unsuspecting.

Statistics may seem like a dry and uninteresting subject, but it’s important to try and get even a rudimentary grasp. The more you know, the less easily you’ll be baffled by mainstream media bullshit.

If you enjoyed this BFD article please consider sharing it with your friends.

Latest